Wednesday 29 April 2015

Imaging studies and visual stress (1)

OK Mr Phillip Jones. You are quite welcome to perform both the SPECT brain scan and the fMRI brain scan in a series of brain scans. Then we can both satisfy our curiosities. Of course I am an Irlen Syndrome sufferer and a severe one at that. Migraines are common place for me without my Irlen tints. But I believe you can't be bothered because you are ultimately arrogant and up yourself. However you are welcome to prove me wrong and then the egg is on my face. Look forward to hearing from you.

So says this individual who posted on a YouTube video about coloured glasses and dyslexia (not migraine). Although she was -looking forward to hearing from me- she disabled the reply function so I was not able to respond. Typical of these people. Although I have not yet got round to looking at visual stress and migraine I will be sure to to do so in future posts. Nonetheless, I thought it would be interesting to look at imaging studies that purport to show brain changes in visual stress.

Background
There is a good deal of concern about the misuse of fMRI and other types of imaging studies of the brain. So much so that there are websites which specifically aim debunk some of the wilder claims of fMRI studies. A good place to start is this section of the Brain Decoder Blog
The problem is that the colours on MRI scans are only statistical maps of blood flow, they are not direct measures of neuronal activity and as such they very prone to statistical artefacts.
fMRI study of a dead salmon (without visual stress)
For example, this fMRI scan of a dead salmon was made using the kind of protocols commonly used in fMRI studies. It clearly shows areas in the salmon's brain lighting up. To counter the problems of low statistical power and imperative to publish, a number of groups are proposing that researchers pre-register their proposed experimental methods and statistical analyses. This should prevent post hoc data trawls which are highly likely to produce false positive results. There is good evidence that this kind of bias is present in the published literature. John Ioannidis has looked at the statistical power of most published studies using fMRI scans and finds far more positive results are being found than would be expected (1) (download here). Clearly, there is a problem afoot. John Ioannidis goes on to suggest that small sample sizes and a flexible approach to data analysis account for the numerous false positive studies which can not be replicated. Unfortunately, the small-scale studies of fMRI scans in visual stress are at a high risk of bias and are at most hypothesis generating.
A second problem is that a positive finding on fMRI studies is not a substitute for finding a real clinical effect. For example, if you found the overlays improved reading but had no effect on fMRI parameters you would still implement treatment. Conversely, if you found changes on the fMRI that were not matched by clinically significant effects so what?
We are treating real people, not fMRI scans.
Of course, there are good reasons why proponents of coloured lenses and overlays promote weak evidence from neuroimaging studies. 
  • Brain branding as it is called - sells. If you can link a product to some 'neuro-sciencey' sounding data particularly if it involves images from fMRI scans it is likely to sell (2).
  • fMRI scans are persuasive. Explanations couched in neuroscientific terms are more likely to be believed and logical shortcoming overlooked(3). Although the findings of this paper have perhaps, in their turn, been overhyped by sceptics.
  • It helps them overlook the weakness of the clinical evidence base.
fMRI studies
So what about fMRI studies in the overlap group visual stress and dyslexia(4,5). The two studies published so far, are at best pilot studies. They do not provide evidence that counterbalances the results from clinical trials that show colour does not alleviate reading difficulties in the overlap group with 'visual stress'. Neither do they provide support for Arnold Wilkins' theory of hyper-excitability of occipital cortical dominance columns. The effects, if real, seem to be confined to the left superior and middle temporal gyri.
In short; fMRI studies do not provide any support for the use of coloured overlays and lenses in individuals with dyslexia and 'visual stress'. In addition, they do not provide support for the speculative neuro-scientific theories used to explain 'visual stress'.

SPECT studies
So what about the SPECT scans mentioned at the beginning of the post? SPECT stands for single photon emission tomography. The spatial resolution is very poor and it is not used for clinical research anymore.
These scans originate from the lucrative Amen clinics run by
TV regular Daniel Amen. They appear where anyone is desperately
plugging Irlen filters.
The images on the left originate for the lucrative private clinic run by Daniel Amen; psychiatrist and TV pundit. According to the president of the American Psychological Association (APA) "in my opinion, what he [Amen] is doing is the modern equivalent of phrenology" and "the claims he makes are not supported by reliable science, and one has to be sceptical about his motivation."
In a consensus statement, the APA states "There are currently no brain imaging biomarkers that are currently clinically useful for any diagnostic category in psychiatry" However, this has not stopped the 'Irlen establishment' from plugging these images.


Conclusion: Neuroimaging studies do not support the use of colour to treat visual stress.





3.         Weisberg DS, Keil FC, Goodstein J, Rawson E, Gray JR. The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008 Mar;20(3):470–7.

4.         Chouinard BD, Zhou CI, Hrybouski S, Kim ES, Cummine J. A Functional Neuroimaging Case Study of Meares–Irlen Syndrome/Visual Stress (MISViS). Brain Topogr. 2012 Jul;25(3):293–307.



No comments:

Post a Comment